The integration of sequential aiming movements: Switching hand and direction at the first target

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

Standard Standard

The integration of sequential aiming movements: Switching hand and direction at the first target. / Lawrence, G.P.; Khan, M.A.; Mottram, T.M. et al.
In: Acta Psychologica, Vol. 164, 29.01.2016, p. 181-87.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

HarvardHarvard

APA

CBE

MLA

VancouverVancouver

Lawrence GP, Khan MA, Mottram TM, Adam JJ, Buckolz E. The integration of sequential aiming movements: Switching hand and direction at the first target. Acta Psychologica. 2016 Jan 29;164:181-87. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.01.005

Author

Lawrence, G.P. ; Khan, M.A. ; Mottram, T.M. et al. / The integration of sequential aiming movements: Switching hand and direction at the first target. In: Acta Psychologica. 2016 ; Vol. 164. pp. 181-87.

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - The integration of sequential aiming movements: Switching hand and direction at the first target

AU - Lawrence, G.P.

AU - Khan, M.A.

AU - Mottram, T.M.

AU - Adam, J.J.

AU - Buckolz, E.

PY - 2016/1/29

Y1 - 2016/1/29

N2 - Movement times to a single target are typically shorter compared to when movement to a second target is required. This one target movement time advantage has been shown to emerge when participants use a single hand throughout the target sequence and when there is a switch between hands at the first target. Our goal was to investigate the lacuna in the movement integration literature surrounding the interactive effects between switching hands and changing movement direction at the first target. Participants performed rapid hand movements in five conditions; movements to a single target; two target movements with a single hand in which the second target required an extension or reversal in direction; and movements to two targets where the hands were switched at the first target and the second target required an extension or reversal in direction. The significance of including these latter two (multiple hand-multiple direction) movements meant that for the first time research could differentiate between peripheral and central processes within movement integration strategies. Reaction times were significantly shorter in the single task compared to the two target tasks. More importantly, movement times to the first target were significantly shorter in the single target task compared to all two target tasks (reflecting the so-called one target advantage), except when the second movement was a reversal movement with the same hand. These findings demonstrate for the first time the contrasting effects of movement integration at central and peripheral levels.

AB - Movement times to a single target are typically shorter compared to when movement to a second target is required. This one target movement time advantage has been shown to emerge when participants use a single hand throughout the target sequence and when there is a switch between hands at the first target. Our goal was to investigate the lacuna in the movement integration literature surrounding the interactive effects between switching hands and changing movement direction at the first target. Participants performed rapid hand movements in five conditions; movements to a single target; two target movements with a single hand in which the second target required an extension or reversal in direction; and movements to two targets where the hands were switched at the first target and the second target required an extension or reversal in direction. The significance of including these latter two (multiple hand-multiple direction) movements meant that for the first time research could differentiate between peripheral and central processes within movement integration strategies. Reaction times were significantly shorter in the single task compared to the two target tasks. More importantly, movement times to the first target were significantly shorter in the single target task compared to all two target tasks (reflecting the so-called one target advantage), except when the second movement was a reversal movement with the same hand. These findings demonstrate for the first time the contrasting effects of movement integration at central and peripheral levels.

U2 - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.01.005

DO - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.01.005

M3 - Article

VL - 164

SP - 181

EP - 187

JO - Acta Psychologica

JF - Acta Psychologica

SN - 0001-6918

ER -