What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

Standard Standard

What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage. / Bested, S.R.; Khan, Michael; Lawrence, Gavin et al.
In: Acta Psychologica, Vol. 186, No. May, 05.2018, p. 110-117.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

HarvardHarvard

Bested, SR, Khan, M, Lawrence, G & Tremblay, L 2018, 'What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage', Acta Psychologica, vol. 186, no. May, pp. 110-117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.03.005

APA

CBE

MLA

VancouverVancouver

Bested SR, Khan M, Lawrence G, Tremblay L. What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage. Acta Psychologica. 2018 May;186(May):110-117. Epub 2018 Apr 3. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.03.005

Author

Bested, S.R. ; Khan, Michael ; Lawrence, Gavin et al. / What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage. In: Acta Psychologica. 2018 ; Vol. 186, No. May. pp. 110-117.

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage

AU - Bested, S.R.

AU - Khan, Michael

AU - Lawrence, Gavin

AU - Tremblay, L.

PY - 2018/5

Y1 - 2018/5

N2 - When moving our upper-limb towards a single target, movement times are typically shorter than when movement to a second target is required. This is known as the one-target advantage. Most studies that have demonstrated the one-target advantage have employed separate trial blocks for the one- and two-segment movements. To test if the presence of the one-target advantage depends on advance knowledge of the number of segments, the present study investigated whether the one-target advantage would emerge under different trial orders/sequences. One- and two-segment responses were organized in blocked (i.e., 1-1-1, 2-2-2), alternating (i.e., 1-2-1-2-1-2), and random (i.e., 1-1-2-1-2-2) trial sequences. Similar to previous studies, where only blocked schedules have typically been utilized, the one-target advantage emerged during the blocked and alternate conditions, but not in the random condition. This finding indicates that the one-target advantage is contingent on participants knowing the number of movement segments prior to stimulus onset.

AB - When moving our upper-limb towards a single target, movement times are typically shorter than when movement to a second target is required. This is known as the one-target advantage. Most studies that have demonstrated the one-target advantage have employed separate trial blocks for the one- and two-segment movements. To test if the presence of the one-target advantage depends on advance knowledge of the number of segments, the present study investigated whether the one-target advantage would emerge under different trial orders/sequences. One- and two-segment responses were organized in blocked (i.e., 1-1-1, 2-2-2), alternating (i.e., 1-2-1-2-1-2), and random (i.e., 1-1-2-1-2-2) trial sequences. Similar to previous studies, where only blocked schedules have typically been utilized, the one-target advantage emerged during the blocked and alternate conditions, but not in the random condition. This finding indicates that the one-target advantage is contingent on participants knowing the number of movement segments prior to stimulus onset.

KW - One-target advantage

KW - Reaction time

KW - Movement constraint hypothesis

KW - Movement integration hypothesis

U2 - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.03.005

DO - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.03.005

M3 - Article

VL - 186

SP - 110

EP - 117

JO - Acta Psychologica

JF - Acta Psychologica

SN - 0001-6918

IS - May

ER -