What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
Standard Standard
In: Acta Psychologica, Vol. 186, No. May, 05.2018, p. 110-117.
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
HarvardHarvard
APA
CBE
MLA
VancouverVancouver
Author
RIS
TY - JOUR
T1 - What’s your number? The effects of trial order on the one-target advantage
AU - Bested, S.R.
AU - Khan, Michael
AU - Lawrence, Gavin
AU - Tremblay, L.
PY - 2018/5
Y1 - 2018/5
N2 - When moving our upper-limb towards a single target, movement times are typically shorter than when movement to a second target is required. This is known as the one-target advantage. Most studies that have demonstrated the one-target advantage have employed separate trial blocks for the one- and two-segment movements. To test if the presence of the one-target advantage depends on advance knowledge of the number of segments, the present study investigated whether the one-target advantage would emerge under different trial orders/sequences. One- and two-segment responses were organized in blocked (i.e., 1-1-1, 2-2-2), alternating (i.e., 1-2-1-2-1-2), and random (i.e., 1-1-2-1-2-2) trial sequences. Similar to previous studies, where only blocked schedules have typically been utilized, the one-target advantage emerged during the blocked and alternate conditions, but not in the random condition. This finding indicates that the one-target advantage is contingent on participants knowing the number of movement segments prior to stimulus onset.
AB - When moving our upper-limb towards a single target, movement times are typically shorter than when movement to a second target is required. This is known as the one-target advantage. Most studies that have demonstrated the one-target advantage have employed separate trial blocks for the one- and two-segment movements. To test if the presence of the one-target advantage depends on advance knowledge of the number of segments, the present study investigated whether the one-target advantage would emerge under different trial orders/sequences. One- and two-segment responses were organized in blocked (i.e., 1-1-1, 2-2-2), alternating (i.e., 1-2-1-2-1-2), and random (i.e., 1-1-2-1-2-2) trial sequences. Similar to previous studies, where only blocked schedules have typically been utilized, the one-target advantage emerged during the blocked and alternate conditions, but not in the random condition. This finding indicates that the one-target advantage is contingent on participants knowing the number of movement segments prior to stimulus onset.
KW - One-target advantage
KW - Reaction time
KW - Movement constraint hypothesis
KW - Movement integration hypothesis
U2 - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.03.005
DO - 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.03.005
M3 - Article
VL - 186
SP - 110
EP - 117
JO - Acta Psychologica
JF - Acta Psychologica
SN - 0001-6918
IS - May
ER -