Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods?

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

Standard Standard

Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods? / Jones, Sam; Brandt, Silke .
In: Journal of Child Language, Vol. 46, No. 6, 11.2019, p. 1260-1273.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

HarvardHarvard

Jones, S & Brandt, S 2019, 'Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods?', Journal of Child Language, vol. 46, no. 6, pp. 1260-1273. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000473

APA

Jones, S., & Brandt, S. (2019). Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods? Journal of Child Language, 46(6), 1260-1273. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000473

CBE

Jones S, Brandt S. 2019. Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods?. Journal of Child Language. 46(6):1260-1273. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000473

MLA

Jones, Sam and Silke Brandt. "Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods?". Journal of Child Language. 2019, 46(6). 1260-1273. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000473

VancouverVancouver

Jones S, Brandt S. Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods? Journal of Child Language. 2019 Nov;46(6):1260-1273. doi: 10.1017/S0305000919000473

Author

Jones, Sam ; Brandt, Silke . / Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods?. In: Journal of Child Language. 2019 ; Vol. 46, No. 6. pp. 1260-1273.

RIS

TY - JOUR

T1 - Do children really acquire dense neighbourhoods?

AU - Jones, Sam

AU - Brandt, Silke

PY - 2019/11

Y1 - 2019/11

N2 - Children learn high phonological neighbourhood density words more easily than low phonological neighbourhood density words (Storkel, 2004). However, the strength of this effect relative to alternative predictors of word acquisition is unclear. We addressed this issue using communicative inventory data from 300 British English-speaking children aged 12 to 25 months. Using Bayesian regression, we modelled word understanding and production as a function of: (i) phonological neighbourhood density, (ii) frequency, (iii) length, (iv) babiness, (v) concreteness, (vi) valence, (vii) arousal, and (viii) dominance. Phonological neighbourhood density predicted word production but not word comprehension, and this effect was stronger in younger children.

AB - Children learn high phonological neighbourhood density words more easily than low phonological neighbourhood density words (Storkel, 2004). However, the strength of this effect relative to alternative predictors of word acquisition is unclear. We addressed this issue using communicative inventory data from 300 British English-speaking children aged 12 to 25 months. Using Bayesian regression, we modelled word understanding and production as a function of: (i) phonological neighbourhood density, (ii) frequency, (iii) length, (iv) babiness, (v) concreteness, (vi) valence, (vii) arousal, and (viii) dominance. Phonological neighbourhood density predicted word production but not word comprehension, and this effect was stronger in younger children.

U2 - 10.1017/S0305000919000473

DO - 10.1017/S0305000919000473

M3 - Article

VL - 46

SP - 1260

EP - 1273

JO - Journal of Child Language

JF - Journal of Child Language

IS - 6

ER -